Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorCaffarra, Sendy
dc.contributor.authorMotamed Haeri, Arman
dc.contributor.authorMichell, Elissa
dc.contributor.authorMartin, Clara D.
dc.date.accessioned2019-11-11T10:19:50Z
dc.date.available2019-11-11T10:19:50Z
dc.date.issued2019
dc.identifier.citationCaffarra S, Motamed Haeri A, Michell E, Martin CD. When is irony influenced by communicative constraints? ERP evidence supporting interactive models. Eur J Neurosci. 2019;50:3566–3577. https ://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.14503es_ES
dc.identifier.issn0953-816X
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10810/36283
dc.descriptionFirst published: 07 July 2019es_ES
dc.description.abstractDistinct theoretical proposals have described how communicative constraints (contextual biases, speaker identity) impact verbal irony processing. Modular models assume that social and contextual factors have an effect at a late stage of processing. Interactive models claim that contextual biases are considered early on. The constraint‐ satisfaction model further assumes that speaker's and context's characteristics can compete at early stages of analysis. The present ERP study teased apart these models by testing the impact of context and speaker features (i.e., speaker accent) on irony analysis. Spanish native speakers were presented with Spanish utterances that were ironic or literal. Each sentence was preceded by a negative or a positive context. Each story was uttered in a native or a foreign accent. Results showed that contextual biases and speaker accent interacted as early as 150 ms during irony processing. Greater N400‐like effects were reported for ironic than literal sentences only with positive contexts and native accent, possibly suggesting semantic difficulties when non‐prototypical irony was produced by natives. A P600 effect of irony was also reported indicating inferential processing costs. The results support the constraintsatisfaction model and suggest that multiple sources of information are weighted and can interact from the earliest stages of irony analysis.es_ES
dc.description.sponsorshipSpanish State Research Agency (Severo Ochoa excellence accreditation), Grant/ Award Number: SEV‐2015‐0490; the Basque Government, Grant/Award Number: BERC 2018–2021, PI_2015_1_25; Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness, Grant/Award Number: PSI2014‐54500, PSI2017‐82941‐P, IJCI‐2016‐27702; Grant/Award Number: H2020‐MSCA‐IF‐2018‐837228. This project has received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement No 837228.es_ES
dc.language.isoenges_ES
dc.publisherEuropean Journal of Neurosciencees_ES
dc.relationinfo:eu-repo/grantAgreement/MINECO/SEV-2015-0490es_ES
dc.relationinfo:eu-repo/grantAgreement/MINECO/PSI2014‐54500es_ES
dc.relationinfo:eu-repo/grantAgreement/MINECO/PSI2017‐82941‐Pes_ES
dc.relationinfo:eu-repo/grantAgreement/MINECO/IJCI‐2016‐27702es_ES
dc.relationinfo:eu-repo/grantAgreement/EC/H2020/MSCA‐IF‐2018‐837228es_ES
dc.rightsinfo:eu-repo/semantics/openAccesses_ES
dc.subjectcontextes_ES
dc.subjectERPes_ES
dc.subjectironyes_ES
dc.subjectspeaker identityes_ES
dc.titleWhen is irony influenced by communicative constraints? ERP evidence supporting interactive modelses_ES
dc.typeinfo:eu-repo/semantics/articlees_ES
dc.rights.holder© 2019 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltdes_ES
dc.relation.publisherversionhttps://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/journal/14609568es_ES
dc.identifier.doi10.1111/ejn.14503


Files in this item

Thumbnail

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record